Aportación de Teoría de la mente de la Batería NEPSY-II como medida de Coherencia central
-
1
Universidad de Navarra
info
ISSN: 1697-5200, 2172-3427
Ano de publicación: 2020
Volume: 18
Número: 3
Páxinas: 277-288
Tipo: Artigo
Outras publicacións en: Revista de investigación en educación
Resumo
Introduction The aim of this research is to analyze the clinical utility of the “Theory of Mind” (TM) subtest of NEPSY-II (Korkman et al., 2014). Methodology TM was applied to 244 school aged children with normal intelligence: a control group (n=194) and clinical groups (n=50): ASD-grade 1 (n= 10); procedural learning disorder (PLD, n=24); and ADHD (n=16). Results and Discussion We do a examination of the factorial structure through a exploratory (KMO=0.62 and MSA=0,54-0,75) and confirmatory (χ =22,75 p=0,504; TLI=1,04; CFI=1; RMSEA=0) analyses. The 21 items of the TM. 3 factors are obtained that explain 62% of the total variance: Theory of mind (items 1/2/7); Social stories comprehension (items 5/8/10/14) and Contextual coherence (items 9/12 / 16-21). The descriptive statistics are provided from the calculation of validity and reliability. None of the groups differ in the first two factors (U-mann-Whitney). In Contextual coherence, the PLD group and the control groups (p=,001) differed significantly of the ASD group (p=,025). The difficulties in understanding information in a quick and simultaneous way and integrating it coherently have been shown to be part of the neuropsychological profile of the PLD. Conclusión Our study provide a new correction an interpretation of the TM that evalautes Central coherence.
Referencias bibliográficas
- Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M. y Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have theory of mind? Cognition, 21, 37-46.
- Berenguer, C. y Roselló, B. (2016). El valor de la Teoría de la Mente en la diferenciación de niños con TDAH y con TEA. En J.L. Castejón (Ed.), Psicología y educación: Presente y futuro(pp. 690-699). Madrid, ACIPE-Asociación Científica de Piscología y Educación
- Bishop, D.V.M. (1998). Development of the Children ́s Communication Checklist (CCC): A method for assessing qualitative impairment in children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and allied disciplines, 39(6), 879-891.
- Bishop, D.V.M. (2003). The Children ́s Communication Checklist second edition (CCC- 2). Londres: The Psychological Corporation.
- Brent, E., Rios, P., Happé, F. y Charman, T. (2004). Performance of children with autism spectrum disorder on advanced theory of mind tasks. Autism, 8, 283-299.
- Browne, M.W. y Cudeck, R. (1989). Single sample cross-validation indices for covariance structures. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 24(4), 445-455.
- Bryne, B.M. (2010). Structural equation modeling with Amos. Basic concepts, applications and programming (2a ed.). Nueva York: Routlege-Taylor and Francis Group.
- Cardillo, R., Garcia, R.B., Mammarella, I.C. y Cornoldi, C. (2018). Pragmatics of language and theory of mind in children with dyslexia with associated language difficulties or nonverbal learning disabilities. Applied Neuropsychology: Child, 7(3), 245-256.
- Cornoldi, C., Fine, J. y Mammarella, I. C. (2016). Nonverbal learning disability. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
- Crespo-Eguílaz, N., Gambra, L., Miguéliz, M., Luna, A., Ruiz, J., Abete, A., Ramos, E., Vergara, M. y Reyes, C. (22-24 de abril de 2020-aplazado). El análisis pormenorizado del Chilhood asperger síndrome test permite diferenciar subtipos clínicos. Comunicación oral. 8th International congress of educational sciences and development. Palacio de Congresos de Pontevedra, España.
- Crespo-Eguílaz, N., Magallón, S., Sánchez-Carpintero, R. y Narbona, J. (2016). La adaptación al castellano de la Children ́s Communication Checklist permite detectar las dificultades en el uso pragmático del lenguaje y diferenciar subtipos clínicos. Revista de Neurología, 62(Supl 1), 49-57.
- Crespo-Eguílaz, N. y Narbona, J. (2009). Trastorno de aprendizaje procedimental: características neuropsicológicas. Revista de Neurología, 49(8), 409-416.
- Crespo-Eguílaz, N., Narbona, J. y Magallón, S. (2012). Disfunción de la coherencia central en niños con trastorno de aprendizaje procedimental. Revista de Neurología, 55(9), 513-519.
- Doty, N. (2019). Nonverbal Learning Disability. En: H. Wilson y E. Braaten (Eds.), The Massachusetts General Hospital Guide to Learning Disabilities. Current Clinical Psychiatry. Boston: Humana Press.
- Drummond, C.R., Ahmad, S.A. y Rourke, B.P. (2005). Rules for the classification of younger children with nonverbal learning disabilities and basic phonological processing disabilities. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20(2), 171-182.
- Gambra, L. (2020). Coherencia central: diseño y validación de una prueba de evaluación y estudio en el trastorno de aprendizaje procedimental (Tesis doctoral). Pamplona: Universidad de Navarra.
- Happé, F. y Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5-25.
- Hu, L. y Bentler, P. (1995). Evaluating model fit. En R. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modelling: Concepts, issues and applications (pp. 76–99). Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications.
- Jolliffe, T.y Baron‐Cohen, S. (1997). Are people with autism and Asperger syndrome faster than normal on the Embedded Figures Test? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 527-534.
- Kline, R.B. (2016). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling(4.a ed.). Nueva York: The Guilford Press.
- Korkman, M., Kirk, U. y Kemp, S. (2014). NEPSY-II: Manual clínico y de interpretación. España: Pearson, The Psychological Corporation.
- Mammarella, I.C., Coltri, S., Lucangeli, D. y Cornoldi, C. (2009). Impairment of simultaneous-spatial working memory in nonverbal (visuospatial) learning disability: A treatment case study. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 19(5), 761-780.
- Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Hubert, B. y Burack, J.A. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 27-43.
- Raven, J.C. (2001). Test de matrices progresivas. Madrid: TEA Ediciones.Rinehart, N.J., Bradshaw, J.L., Moss, S.A., Brereton, A.V. y Tonge, B.J. (2000). Atypical interference of local detail on global processing in high-functioning autism and Asperger's disorder. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41(6), 769-778.
- Sans, A., Colomé, R., López-Sala, A. y Boix, C. (2009). Trastorno de aprendizaje no verbal: características cognitivo-conductuales y aspectos neuropsicológicos.
- Schermelleh-Engel, K., Moosbrugger, H. y Müller, H. (2003). Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: Tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods of Psychological Research Online, 8(2), 23–74.
- Scott, F.J., Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P. y Brayne, C. (2002). The CAST (Childhood Asperger Syndrome Test) Preliminary development of a UK screen for mainstream primary-school-age children. Autism, 6(1), 9-31.
- Semrud-Clikeman, M., Walkowiak, J., Wilkinson, A.y Christopher, G. (2010). Neuropsychological differences among children with Asperger syndrome, nonverbal learning disabilities, attention deficit disorder, and controls. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35(5), 582-600.